
Ease Comparisons of Your Price
Price Psychology
Ease Comparisons of Your Price
Prices are compared to past prices, competitors, and adjacent numbers.
How do customers evaluate prices?
They use reference prices (see Mazumdar et al., 2005).
Imagine a carton of eggs for $5.00.
Is $5.00 a good deal? How can you tell? What happens in your brain?
You compare $5.00 to a variety of numbers:
- Past Prices. You paid $4.50 last time.
- Advertised Prices. A flyer said $3.49.
- Estimated Prices. You expected $4.00.
- Competitor Prices. Other eggs are $2.99.
- Ideal Price. You'd like to pay $3.50.
- Maximum Price: You won't pay more than $5.50.
- Nearby Numbers. You read 100% Organic.
And other numbers (see Lowengart, 2002)
You merge those sizes into a single magnitude, say $4.00, then compare this magnitude to the base price of $5.00.
Hmm, in this case, you'd need to pay more.
But that's okay. If this difference still feels reasonable, you'll buy the eggs and feel a slight twinge of pain that reduces your spending in the remaining shopping trip.
How to Apply
- Compare Yearly Prices When You're Cheaper. Customers are biased toward absolute numbers: The relative difference between 5 and 9 feels smaller than 500 and 900 (Pandelaere et al., 2011). Therefore, don't compare your $5/month plan to a $9/month alternative. Compare the yearly differences at $60/year and $108/year. The absolute difference is now $48 instead $4.
- Compare All Your Prices to Competitors, or No Prices. Selective comparisons feel suspicious. Any price without a comparison is assumed to be higher (Barone et al., 2004).
- Name Competitors in Comparisons. "Seen Elsewhere for $50" feels suspicious (Krishnan et al., 2006).
- Bundle When Competitors Are Cheaper. Prevent direct comparisons with a cheaper competitor (Balachander et al., 2010).
- Place Your Price Underneath Competitors. You'll benefit either way: You'll ease subtraction when cheaper, yet hinder subtraction when more expensive (Guha et al., 2018).
- Balachander, S., Ghosh, B., & Stock, A. (2010). Why bundle discounts can be a profitable alternative to competing on price promotions. Marketing Science, 29(4), 624-638.
- Barone, M. J., Manning, K. C., & Miniard, P. W. (2004). Consumer response to retailers’ use of partially comparative pricing. Journal of Marketing, 68(3), 37-47.
- Briesch, R. A., Krishnamurthi, L., Mazumdar, T., & Raj, S. P. (1997). A comparative analysis of reference price models. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 202-214.
- Grewal, D., Marmorstein, H., & Sharma, A. (1996). Communicating price information through semantic cues: the moderating effects of situation and discount size. Journal of Consumer research, 23(2), 148-155.
- Grewal, D., Roggeveen, A. L., & Lindsey-Mullikin, J. (2014). The contingent effects of semantic price cues. Journal of Retailing, 90(2), 198-205.
- Krishnan, B. C., Biswas, A., & Netemeyer, R. G. (2006). Semantic cues in reference price advertisements: The moderating role of cue concreteness. Journal of Retailing, 82(2), 95-104.
- Lowengart, O. (2002). Reference price conceptualisations: An integrative framework of analysis. Journal of Marketing Management, 18(1-2), 145-171.
- Mazumdar, T., Raj, S. P., & Sinha, I. (2005). Reference price research: Review and propositions. Journal of marketing, 69(4), 84-102.
- Pandelaere, M., Briers, B., & Lembregts, C. (2011). How to make a 29% increase look bigger: The unit effect in option comparisons. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(2), 308-322.

Want more tactics?
Get all my free pricing tactics